Trending Topics

Bodycam vs dash cam: Which might give you the best break in court?

New research reveals that people assign blame differently after viewing bodycam versus dash cam footage

body_worn_camera.jpg

Because of the weight the public gives to video evidence in assessing police “accountability,” this finding could have significant legal and public relations implications.

Photo/PoliceOne

This article is reprinted with permission from the Calibre Report by Calibre Press. To subscribe, email editor@calibrepress.com.

Civilians tend to judge an officer’s actions more leniently when they see a body-worn camera recording of an incident than when they watch the same event on dash cam footage, according to a Northwestern U. research team.

Because of the weight the public gives to video evidence in assessing police “accountability” these days, this finding could have significant legal and public relations implications.

Indeed, in one experiment volunteers acting as mock grand jurors were asked to decide whether an officer should be indicted for an encounter on a traffic incident that unintentionally turned bad. Those who watched the incident on dash cam were highly more likely to recommend a charge of assault, battery, or aggravated battery than those who viewed footage from the officer’s body cam.

There appears to be a “diminished sense of blame or responsibility for the person who’s wearing the body cam,” says Dr. Neal Roese, an NU professor both of psychology and marketing and an internationally recognized expert on the psychology of judgment and decision-making. A body cam wearer’s intentions seem to be regarded differently, he explains – an important distinction in the legal arena, where intentionality can be a pivotal consideration.

A full account of the researchers’ work has been published under the title “Body camera footage leads to lower judgments of intent than dash camera footage” in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. A layman’s synopsis of the findings from a Northwestern newsletter is available here.

Unstudied consequences

The research was initiated by Broderick Turner, a PhD candidate at Northwestern’s Kellogg School of Management, who in his private life had developed a curiosity about the pervasiveness and power of surveillance video in controversial police encounters.

“Despite the widespread use” of bodycams and dash cams, he writes, “little is known about [their] specific impact on judgments.” Because “most police videos used in court depict negative outcomes,” there’s an obvious motivation “to avoid blame for [that] outcome.” But in that effort, he wondered, is all footage equal? Or might video from different types of cameras be perceived differently? And might these differences affect how an officer’s actions are evaluated?

Along with Professor Roese and two other researchers, Turner recruited an online test pool of about 2,000 U.S.-based civilian volunteers and exposed random groups of them to different experiments involving real-world police videos from YouTube, plus a few staged films created specifically for the study. Across eight experiments, 26 videos total were involved, all played without sound.

Varied scenarios/same outcome

One test, for example, involved two real-life films in which an officer shoots a suspect and one in which an officer breaks a window of a driver’s car. Dash cam and bodycam versions of each incident were shown, edited to have the same start-start moments and to be of identical duration.

Each volunteer then rated the involved officer on a sliding scale as to:

  • Whether he/she acted intentionally (from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”);
  • How much blame the officer deserves (from “none” to “a great deal”);
  • How much the officer should be punished (from “not at all” to “a great deal”).

Results were consistent. Viewers who saw the bodycam footage judged the officer as “less intentional,” thought he deserved less blame, and called for lighter punishment than those who saw the dash cam video.

Why?

Seeking an explanation, the researchers initially speculated that perhaps “bodycam footage invites the viewer to take the wearer’s perspective,” that is, seeing the action from the officer’s point of view might create an empathy that favors the officer.

But further experimentation led the team to a much different – and less logical – conclusion. What really seemed to make the difference, the study found, is that “bodycams typically contain fewer visual indicators of the focal actor [the wearing officer], which decreases attention to that actor.” Dash cams, with their “third-person perspective,” tend to render “the actor’s body more visually prominent.”

In identifying the importance of what the researchers refer to as “visual salience,” they created videos in which bodycam wearers were “overtly intentional” – tipping things over, dropping things, kicking things, etc. Some of these videos showed no part of the camera wearer’s body, while in other versions the wearer’s arms or feet were visible.

Where viewers could see sustained parts of the wearer on bodycam footage, the bodycam lost its judgmental edge over the dash cam regarding intentionality. In short, the usual difference in viewer reaction to bodycam vs. dash cam was “mitigated.”

That may be, the study speculates, because showing body parts can reveal an officer’s dynamic movement and convey “additional information about how the incident unfolds in real time, including subtle cues as to the [officer’s] mental state.”

“Jurors” speak

Of particular interest in the study is the mock jury experiment. Here about 200 volunteers pretending to be grand jurors considered a real-life case in which an officer approached a vehicle stopped in traffic, with a driver who apparently had passed out. Unable to rouse the driver, the officer broke a passenger-side window, “startling the driver, who then accelerated suddenly and crashed” into a power pole, starting a fire.

All participants read the officer’s report. Some also watched bodycam footage of the incident, while some others watched a dash cam recording. When all then voted on whether to indict the officer on criminal charges, the differences were striking:

  • Over 70% of the dash cam viewers wanted to send the officer to trial for assault, while less than half of the bodycam group voted to;
  • Nearly 70% of dash cam viewers favored indictment for battery, compared to 53% of the bodycam group;
  • Sixty percent of the dash cam watchers called for a charge of aggravated battery vs. less than half of those who saw bodycam footage.

Interestingly, “jurors” who only read the officer’s official report but saw no video tended also to be significantly more likely to vote for indictment.

“When bodycam [footage] is included, the mean odds of indictment decrease,” the researchers conclude. “[T]his research suggests that viewing bodycam footage might make judgments by jurors and as well by the general public more lenient toward the bodycam wearer [and] might in some cases reduce the likelihood that grand juries will indict a police officer, compared with dash cam video or a written report of the incident.”

The team emphasizes, however, that more research on video impact is urgently needed: “For society to benefit most from the greater transparency conferred by emerging forms of surveillance, these advances in technology require corresponding advances in our understanding of their effects on observant judgment.”

Thanks to Lt. Glenn Mills, president of the Massachusetts Assn. of Crime Analysts for alerting us to this study.

The Calibre Column is provided by Calibre Press, Inc., one of the most recognized and respected law enforcement training organizations in the industry. Offering hundreds of courses each year on topics ranging from Street Survival®, Active Threat Engagement and Assault Detection & Response to Highway Patrol Tactics, Emotional Survival, and Leadership, Calibre Press has trained more than one million officers nationwide over its 40-year history. In addition to on-site courses, Calibre also offers an increasing collection of training-focused videos available through www.calibrepress.com.

Contact Calibre Press.

RECOMMENDED FOR YOU